The rationale was clear.
“Most people go about their daily life assuming that they have more control over their behavior than they actually do,” wrote a young psychology professor at Stanford University in 1971. “We are often unaware of the tremendous power which social situations exert upon us to shape, guide, and manipulate our behavior.”
To prove these statements, the professor devised an experiment.
Together with 24 male graduate students, he built a prison in the basement of one of the university’s buildings. Its cells measured 6 feet by 9 feet. By a coin toss, students were divided into prisoners and guards. The professor was the warden.
Within a day, the students (“seemingly gentle and caring young men,” as they were later described) found themselves pitted against one another. The guards became mean and tyrannical, the prisoners sad, angry and withdrawn.
The study was intended to last for two weeks. It was shut down after six days, but its implications have had a profound effect ever since.
Chief architect of this landmark and controversial study — known as the “Stanford Prison Experiment” — was Philip G. Zimbardo, who died Oct. 14 at his home in San Francisco, according to Stanford University. He was 91.
Praised as “one of the most prolific and influential psychologists of his generation” by a Stanford colleague, Zimbardo was also criticized for the active, non-objective role he played in the experiment. But while his methodology was questioned, he opened the door for a generation of social scientists for the analysis of often subjective and inchoate aspects of human emotion and behavior.
Intrigued by shyness, Zimbardo founded the Stanford Shyness Clinic to uncover the roots of anxiety, panic and social phobia. Intrigued by altruism, he established the Heroic Imagination Project, devoted to the belief — in his own words — that “each and every seemingly ordinary person on this planet is capable of committing heroic acts.”
He was president of the American Psychological Association in 2002, and after retiring was honored for his volunteer service to Stanford. He was the recipient of the 2006 Havel Foundation Prize for outstanding work in the field of science and awarded the Richard W. Lyman award for his “contributions to the development and integration of psychological research and social action.”
Born in New York City on March 23, 1933, Zimbardo grew up in the Bronx. Eldest of four children, he attended Brooklyn College and Yale University, where he earned a master’s degree in experimental psychology and a doctorate in social psychology. After teaching at New York University and Columbia University, he joined the Stanford faculty in 1968.
At the time, psychology was increasingly popular and influential field of inquiry. The writings of Erik Erickson, R.D. Laing and D.W. Winnicott were rapidly changing presumptions about the development of the human mind.
Zimbardo’s “Introduction to Psychology” class was so popular that lines often formed outside offices and ran into the university quadrangles of students trying to get admitted.
Arguing that social structures — from the workplace to schools, neighborhoods to marriages — can effect a range of behavior, Zimbardo showed that when people felt anonymous and superior and believed they could act with impunity, they would disregard the well-being of others.
As a young researcher, he once parked a beat-up car in a middle-class suburban neighborhood of Palo Alto, and during three days of observation, noticed that the car was left untouched. Destructive behavior, he concluded, is discouraged by a sense of community and a feeling of social disapproval.
Conversely destructive behavior proliferates in communities that condone it.
After the 1978 mass murder and suicide of 909 people in the Jonestown settlement in Guyana directed by the charismatic leader Jim Jones, Zimbardo explored the social dynamics of cults. After calls for inquiry into the abuse of Iraqi prisoners by American soldiers at the Abu Ghraib prison, Zimbardo was named to the commission.
His work has continuing implications for the study of “extreme forms of influence,” such as terrorist recruiting, cults and “human malleability or resiliency when confronted by authority power.”
“Understanding the dynamics and pervasiveness of situational power is essential to learning how to resist it,” he wrote in 2002, “and to weaken the dominance of the many agents of mind control who ply their trade daily on all of us behind many faces and fronts.”
But his work in the mock prison in the basement of a building on the Stanford campus defined his reputation.
Writing 25 years after the Stanford Prison Experiment, he and coauthor, Craig Haney, a fellow researcher in 1971, were candid about how “shocking and unexpected” the outcome was: transforming “most of the participants in ways we did not anticipate, prepare for or predict.”
As much as the academic community and general public were left reeling by their conclusions, Zimbardo understood their importance for showing that psychological research could be applied to everyday life and understood and appreciated by nonprofessionals.
He passionately argued that psychology was relevant to the national dialogue about crime and justice and deplored the “politicians and policymakers [who] now seem, to worship the very kind of institutional power whose adverse effects were so critically evaluated over the past 25 years.”
By applying the methods and theories of modern psychology to the contextual origins of crime and the pain of imprisonment, he hoped for “genuine and meaningful prison and criminal justice reform.”
“There has never been a more critical time at which to begin the intellectual struggle with those who would demean human nature by using prisons exclusively as agencies of social control that punish without attempting to rehabilitate, that isolate and oppress instead of educating and elevating, and that tear down minority communities rather than protecting and strengthening them.”